zaangazowanie poznawcze_cośtam_dzieci_ADHD.pdf

(144 KB) Pobierz
92861850 UNPDF
2006, Vol. 42, No. 6, 1206–1219
Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
Cognitive Engagement and Story Comprehension in Typically Developing
Children and Children With ADHD From Preschool
Through Elementary School
Elizabeth P. Lorch, Richard Milich, Clarese C. Astrin, and Kristen S. Berthiaume
University of Kentucky
The present study examined children’s cognitive engagement with television as a function of the
continuity of central or incidental content and whether this varied with age and clinical status. In
Experiment 1, 9- to 11-year-old children’s response times on a secondary task were slower the later a
probe occurred in a sequence of central events, and response times predicted recall. Experiment 2
extended these results to 6- to 8-year-old children. Experiment 3 revealed that children with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) failed to show the pattern consistently observed for comparison
children. The results support the hypothesis that typically developing children build a representation
during viewing that reflects the causal structure of the televised story but that this skill is deficient in 4-
to 9-year-old children with ADHD.
Keywords: ADHD, cognitive engagement, secondary probe, attention
Supplemental data: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1206.supp
Knowledge of children’s story comprehension has been in-
formed by theories of the comprehension process. A theme com-
mon to most theories is an emphasis on the causal and enabling
relations between events in a story, which define plot-relevant
events and give the story coherence (Ackerman, 1986; Black &
Bower, 1980; Graesser & Clark, 1985; Mandler & Johnson, 1977;
Trabasso, Secco, & van den Broek, 1984; van den Broek, 1997).
Characteristics of the causal structure of stories have been found to
predict a product of children’s comprehension—that is, memory
for story events (e.g., Trabasso et al., 1984; van den Broek, 1989;
van den Broek, Lorch, & Thurlow, 1996)—with effects becoming
stronger with age (Nezworski, Stein, & Trabasso, 1982; Schmidt &
Paris, 1983; van den Broek et al., 1996). However, much less
research has investigated the online processes children may engage
in to detect and use causal relations between events to build a
representation of a story (Ackerman, Paine, & Silver, 1991; Tra-
basso & Nickels, 1992; Trabasso, Stein, Rodkin, Munger, &
Baughn, 1992). One avenue of research that may contribute to
knowledge of children’s online story comprehension processes is
the study of systematic variations in children’s attention to tele-
vised stories (Anderson & Lorch, 1983; Huston & Wright, 1983).
The present series of studies builds on this line of research by
using a measure of moment-to-moment cognitive capacity usage to
investigate several questions. First, does children’s cognitive en-
gagement with a televised story vary as a function of the contin-
uation of plot-relevant content? Second, does this pattern differ as
a function of age? Finally, do children with documented attention
problems and story comprehension difficulties (i.e., diagnosed
with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD]) differ in
cognitive engagement from comparison children?
Influences on Children’s Attention to Television
Several theoretical viewpoints on children’s television viewing
concur that children, from an early age, are active viewers whose
visual attention to television is guided by ongoing comprehension,
expectations, and purposes for viewing (Anderson & Lorch, 1983;
Huston & Wright, 1983; Salomon, 1983). For example, Anderson
and Lorch (1983) proposed that for young children, a look at the
television may begin in response to any of several possible rea-
sons, including stimuli that elicit orienting responses; formal fea-
tures that signal informative, appealing, child-relevant content
(Alwitt, Anderson, Lorch, & Levin, 1980); and cues derived from
the behavior of other children (Anderson, Lorch, Smith, Bradford,
& Levin, 1981). Once a look has begun, its continuation primarily
depends on the child’s ongoing judgments of whether program
content is comprehensible (Anderson, Lorch, Field, & Sanders,
1981; Lorch, Anderson, & Levin, 1979; Pingree, 1986).
Huston and Wright (1983) discussed similar factors as influ-
ences on visual attention but conceptualized a series of decisions
children make about whether to continue a look at the television.
At the beginning of a look, decisions are most heavily a function
of formal characteristics, once again those that are indicative of
informative, interesting, child-relevant content (Calvert, Huston,
Watkins, & Wright, 1982; Huston et al., 1981). If a look continues,
more extensive, deeper cognitive processing takes place, such that
a subsequent decision is affected by both current comprehension
Elizabeth P. Lorch, Richard Milich, Clarese C. Astrin, and Kristen S.
Berthiaume, Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky.
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health
Grant MH47386.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Elizabeth
P. Lorch, Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington,
KY 40506-0044. E-mail: elorch@uky.edu
1206
Developmental Psychology
0012-1649/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1206
COGNITIVE ENGAGEMENT WITH TELEVISION
1207
and initial expectations about content (Campbell, Wright, & Hus-
ton, 1987; Rolandelli, Wright, Huston, & Eakins, 1991). As further
cycles occur, later decisions are increasingly influenced by deeper
processing and more elaborated expectations about story content
(Hawkins, Tapper, Bruce, & Pingree, 1995; Rolandelli et al.,
1991). In addition, Huston and Wright (1983) proposed that there
are changes in these cycles of decisions as development advances.
For younger children, the continuation of a look at the television
may be more dependent on superficial features of the program than
for older children, who are more likely to make elaborated deci-
sions on the basis of deeper processing of content (Rolandelli et
al., 1991).
These perspectives and their associated empirical support indi-
cate that children’s visual attention to television varies systemat-
ically in relation to program characteristics. Huston and Wright’s
(1983) perspective also suggests a context for the online process-
ing of television story structure. To the extent that children build
an ongoing representation of the interrelations among story events
during viewing, measures of attention may reveal effects of event
centrality, causal structure, and plot development.
Several investigations suggest such indications of children’s
online processing of structural features of television stories.
School-age children have been found to look at the television more
during presentation of material that adults rated as important (i.e.,
central) to the plot of the story than during material rated as low in
importance (Baer & Lorch, 1990; Lorch & Baer, 1997). Mead-
owcroft and Reeves (1989) obtained a related finding using a
secondary task technique to assess cognitive engagement with the
television program. The secondary task technique is based on the
assumptions that mental processing requires time and that central
processing capacity is limited (Basil, 1994; Kahneman, 1973).
Thus, the more attentional capacity is engaged by a continuous
primary task, such as viewing a television program, the less is
available for a secondary task, such as a keypress in response to an
occasional auditory tone (Britton, Graesser, Glynn, Hamilton, &
Penland, 1983; Britton & Tesser, 1982; Thorson, Reeves, &
Schleuder, 1985). Meadowcroft and Reeves (1989) found that 5- to
8-year-old children who had tested high in the development of
story schema skills showed greater cognitive engagement (i.e.,
slower responses to secondary probes) with central content pre-
sented in a normal story structure than with the same content
presented out of order and thus in the absence of a coherent story
structure.
Two additional studies also compared children’s responses to
normally structured, coherent stories and stories in which scenes
had been edited out of sequence, via existing edit points (Hawkins,
Kim, & Pingree, 1991; Lorch & Castle, 1997). The use of existing
edit points allowed the edited stories to be locally comprehensible
but lacking in a coherent story structure. Both studies used the
same types of Sesame Street stories, and in both studies children
showed high visual attention to normal stories and to edited stories.
Both studies, however, revealed indications that children’s atten-
tion was engaged more systematically by the normally structured
stories. Lorch and Castle (1997), using the secondary task meth-
odology, found that 5-year-old children showed greater cognitive
engagement during the second half of normal stories than during
the first half, but the authors did not observe this difference for the
stories shown in edited form. Hawkins et al. (1991) investigated
the predictability in 3- to 6-year-old children’s visual attention
from one 3-s interval to the next (after removal of variance
resulting from overall attention, age, and tape version). Consistent
with the results of Lorch and Castle (1997), Hawkins et al. (1991)
found much higher stability in attention late in normal stories than
in edited stories. Taken together, the findings of Hawkins et al.
(1991), Lorch and Castle (1997), and Meadowcroft and Reeves
(1989) suggest that children’s attention is responsive to the plot
relevance of story content and becomes increasingly engaged as a
meaningful narrative structure develops.
The major purpose of the current studies is to extend investiga-
tion of school-age children’s online cognitive processing of tele-
vision story content by examining whether children’s cognitive
engagement with a television program increases as sequences of
story content that are central to the plot continue or decreases as
sequences of content that are incidental to the plot continue. In
addition, Experiment 2 investigates whether there are developmen-
tal differences in children’s cognitive engagement as a function of
the continuity of central or incidental content.
Finally, Experiment 3 examines the cognitive engagement of
children with ADHD, whose problems in sustaining attention and
comprehending causal relations have been well documented
(Lorch, Eastham, et al., 2004; Milich, Lorch, & Berthiaume,
2005). Two studies have investigated directly the role of cognitive
engagement in story comprehension among children with ADHD.
Lorch, Eastham, et al. (2004) used a television viewing method-
ology to test the hypothesis that differences in cognitive engage-
ment would account for group differences in recall of causal
relations (i.e., “why” questions) when toys were present during
viewing. The results of three different analytic strategies con-
verged to support the hypothesis that greater cognitive engage-
ment, as indexed by long looks (i.e., longer than 15 s) at the
television, enabled comparison children to form a more complete
representation of the relations among story events, thereby ac-
counting for differences between the comparison and ADHD
groups in understanding causal relations questions. Thus, the find-
ings from Lorch, Eastham, et al. (2004) constitute the first com-
pelling evidence that the amount of time spent in deeper cognitive
processing during long looks helps explain the differential patterns
of recall in children with ADHD and comparison children.
The results of Lorch, Eastham, et al. (2004) suggest that varia-
tions in cognitive engagement may account for the problems that
children with ADHD have with causal relations questions. How-
ever, the degree of cognitive engagement was inferred from time
spent in long looks. The secondary task may offer a more direct
way to assess cognitive engagement. To date, only one study has
used this procedure with an ADHD sample (Whirley, Lorch,
Lemberger, & Milich, 2003). In this study, participants were 22
boys with ADHD and 36 comparison boys, ranging in age from 9
to 11 years. Boys with ADHD responded significantly slower than
the comparison boys, a common finding in reaction time studies.
More important, the patterns of response times (RTs) across the
central sequences differed between the two groups of boys. The
comparison boys showed the predicted pattern of longer RTs (i.e.,
increased cognitive engagement) the longer into a central sequence
the probes appeared. In contrast, the boys with ADHD actually
showed shorter RTs from the beginning to the middle of the central
sequences, which suggests decreased cognitive engagement as the
central sequences progressed. It was only for probes occurring late
in the central sequences that the RTs of the boys with ADHD
1208
LORCH, MILICH, ASTRIN, AND BERTHIAUME
showed the expected increase. In Experiment 3, we use a devel-
opmental perspective to compare the cognitive engagement of
children with ADHD and their comparison peers, using an age
range that included younger children (i.e., 4 to 9 years) than the
previous secondary task studies.
To address whether children’s cognitive engagement is related
to the development of content that varies in plot relevance, pro-
grams were selected that contained strong narrative structures,
with story lines revolving around child characters, and that enabled
the identification of continuous sequences (approximately 15 s or
more) of events that were central or incidental to the plot. Central
sequences are those that are crucial to plot development, whereas
incidental sequences are ones that could be removed without
affecting the coherence of the story. Central and incidental se-
quences of events were operationalized in terms of the combina-
tion of two criteria: whether events in the sequence were part of the
causal chain leading from the beginning to the eventual outcome of
the story (as opposed to “dead end” events), and whether events
were high or low in centrality, as determined by college student
raters (see Experiment 1 Method section for greater detail). For
each central or incidental sequence of events, several positions
were identified as potential times for presentation of secondary
probes. Watching and understanding the television program was
defined as the primary task, and children were told that their
knowledge of story events would be tested after viewing. As a
child watched the program, auditory probes were presented at
preselected times. The child’s secondary task was to press a key as
quickly as possible whenever a probe was presented.
Within the constraints of this task, visual attention was expected
to be very high, so the cycles of viewing decisions described by
Huston and Wright (1983) were not expected to occur. Probe RTs,
however, may capture similar decisions concerning whether to
engage in deeper and more elaborative processing. If children
build a representation during viewing that reflects the causal
structure of a televised story, they should show increasing cogni-
tive engagement as a sequence of central story content continues,
because they will encounter events that can be related to the main
thread of the story. Therefore, the later a probe was presented in a
sequence of central events, the slower children’s responses were
expected to be. In contrast, this pattern was not expected to occur
as a sequence of incidental events continued. The lack of connec-
tions to the causal chain of the story should lead to more superfi-
cial processing. Thus, children’s RTs might be unrelated to probe
position or might even become shorter the later a probe was
presented in a sequence of incidental events.
had ever been referred for any attentional or behavioral difficulties. Children
were paid $5 for their participation, which lasted about 45 min. Data were lost
from 2 participants as a result of equipment malfunction.
Materials
Each participant viewed one episode of the situation comedy Growing
Pains . This program was chosen specifically because its content is suitable
and interesting for children. Furthermore, the plot of this specific episode
(“Dad’s Birthday”) centered on the activities of one of the child characters
in the family. A synopsis of the plot appears in the Appendix, which is
available on the Web. With all commercials removed, the episode is 23 min
in length. A detailed audiovisual script of the program was created as part
of a previous study (Lorch et al., 2000). The script was divided into 407
individual units of meaning, with each unit representing a single idea or event.
In accordance with procedures defined by Trabasso and van den Broek
(1985), a causal network representation of the story was derived. On the
basis of the causal network analysis, each idea unit was coded as to whether
it was on or off the causal chain. The causal chain is a sequence of events
that are causally linked together and carry the story from the beginning to
the end. All story events are either part of the causal chain (i.e., causally
connected to prior and subsequent events) or dead-end events (i.e., not
causally connected to prior and subsequent events on the causal chain).
Each idea unit also had been rated by 58 college students for its
centrality to the story, on a scale from 1 to 7. The students first watched the
program and then were given scripts representing the individual units in the
program. In assigning their ratings, they were instructed to consider how
much plot-relevant information the unit conveyed, how much would be lost
if the unit were removed from the program, and how much the unit
enhanced understanding of the plot. Mean centrality ratings were calcu-
lated across all students for each idea unit.
For the purposes of the present study, an individual unit was considered
to be central if it both was on the causal chain and had a mean centrality
rating of at least 5.15 out of 7, the upper quartile of centrality ratings. An
individual unit was considered to be incidental if it both was off the causal
chain (a dead-end event) and had a mean centrality rating of no more than
3.30, the lower quartile of ratings. Given these classifications, nine con-
tinuous sequences of primarily central events and nine continuous se-
quences of primarily incidental events were identified. A description of
each sequence and its order of appearance in the Growing Pains episode
appears in the Appendix (available on the Web). The length of the se-
quences varied from 15 to 90 s, and sequences contained between 4 and 30
idea units. In central sequences, the mean centrality rating of the units was
5.53, 77% of the individual units were in the upper quartile of the ratings,
and 72.4% of the individual units were on the causal chain. In incidental
sequences, the mean centrality rating of the units was 2.88, 85% of the
individual units were in the lower quartile of the ratings, and none of the
individual units were on the causal chain. In general, individual units in
either type of sequence that did not meet strict criteria for the sequence
tended to be brief and isolated.
To track online variations in children’s cognitive engagement with the
television story, we placed 26 auditory target probes at preselected points
during these sequences of story events. The basic positions for target
probes were approximately 2, 7, or 12 s into the sequence. 1 In addition, in
Experiment 1
Method
Participants
Participants were 27 boys and 33 girls, ages 9–11 years ( M 10.05, SD
0.81). Participants were primarily Caucasian (88.33%), with some African
American children (8.33%) and children of other ethnic identifications
(3.33%). Most parents’ education included at least some college; the average
parent was a college graduate ( M 16.98 years of education). Children were
recruited through advertisements in the newspaper and from an existing pool
of experimental volunteers. For Experiment 1, children were screened for
behavioral problems or attentional difficulties, such as ADHD, in a recruitment
phone call and were not included in the study if the parent indicated the child
1 The nine central and nine incidental sequences exhausted all possible
sequences of story events that met criteria for classification as central or
incidental and continued for a minimum of 15 s. Fifteen seconds was
chosen as the minimum because this is the asymptote of the attentional
inertia function (Anderson & Lorch, 1983). The basic probe positions of 2,
7, and 12 s were chosen to represent early, middle, and later positions
within the 15-s interval. The number of later probes was constrained by the
number of sequences longer than 24 s.
COGNITIVE ENGAGEMENT WITH TELEVISION
1209
sequences lasting 24 s or longer, later probe positions were added to the
basic probes. We created three series of probe assignments to counterbal-
ance probe position across sequences. Each participant was randomly
assigned to one of these three series. Each series contained 3 probes in each
basic position (i.e., 2, 7, and 12 s), at each centrality level, and 4 probes in
the later positions during sequences longer than 24 s, for a total of 13
central and 13 incidental target probes. We inserted 12 filler probes in the
program to prevent participants from detecting a pattern or being able to
predict probes and make anticipatory responses, which resulted in a total of
38 probes during the episode.
A set of comprehension questions was developed for the television
program. The questions tested factual information that was presented in the
program during sequences containing probes. Discrete events that occurred
close in time to a secondary task probe were chosen to be tested, and
straightforward questions were designed to test children’s memory for each
event. An example question testing memory for a central story event (see
Appendix, available on the Web) is “Ben has to return the camera. What
else does he have to do as part of his punishment?” (return money and
apologize to neighbors). Twenty-five cued recall questions were devel-
oped; 14 tested central content, and 11 tested incidental content. Every
sequence was represented by at least 1 question, with two sequences
represented by 2 questions, two represented by 3 questions, and two
represented by 4 questions.
After the child indicated verbally that he or she understood the instructions
and had satisfactorily completed the practice probe program, the experi-
mental session began. The experimenter started the videotape that played
the television show and synchronized the probe program to the tape before
leaving the room.
When the television program was finished, the experimenter reentered
the room and began the cued recall questioning session. The testing session
was videotaped to allow later scoring of cued recall questions. The ques-
tions were asked in the order in which the content was presented in the
show. If the child did not answer a question correctly, the experimenter
supplied the correct answer and then continued to the next question until all
questions were asked. The child was then debriefed, paid, and thanked for
his or her participation.
Results and Discussion
Dependent Measures
The main dependent variable was RT to 26 target probes.
Thirty-eight probes sounded during the program. Thirteen target
probes occurred during sequences of central content, and 13 oc-
curred during sequences of incidental content. The remaining
probes were fillers and were not analyzed. Two participants’ RT
data included one impossibly fast RT (32 ms for 1 child, 112 ms
for the other). These responses were not included in the calculation
of means for the categories. Two participants failed to respond to
1 probe. We obtained category means for these participants by
computing means of the remaining RTs in the category.
Performance on the cued recall questions was scored from
transcripts of the videotape. Each answer was assigned a score of
1 for correct and 0 for incorrect. An independent rater scored 25%
of participants’ cued recall responses, with 92% agreement. The
proportion correct was computed for each participant separately
for central and incidental questions.
Participants’ visual attention to the television was coded from
videotapes. Using a computer program synchronized to the begin-
ning of the television program, we obtained a continuous record of
the child’s looks at and away from the television. This continuous
record of looks allowed for identification of looking status at the
time of every probe as either on or off task. The mean percentage
of visual attention was at ceiling ( M
Procedure
The participants were brought to the on-campus television viewing
facility by a parent. Each child participated individually. Before the ex-
periment began, informed consent was obtained from the parent. After a
brief conversation with the experimenter to help the child feel comfortable
(e.g., talking to the child about school, activities, pets), the child was shown
to his or her seat in the television viewing room. The child was seated at
a table, with a computer keyboard on the table in front of him or her. The
probes were generated by an IBM computer that was located on the floor
next to the child. Responses were made on the keyboard on the table in
front of the child. RTs for each probe were recorded (in milliseconds) from
the beginning of the probe until the child responded by pressing the space
bar. The probe continued until a response was made. If no response was
made, the sound ended after 5 s.
A television was situated on a desk near one corner of the table, with the
television screen about 5 ft (1.5 m) from the child’s seat. A video camera
was mounted on the wall in a position that allowed the image to include
both the child and the television. For the child to look at the television, he
or she needed to make a noticeable head movement. This enabled the
experimenter to record looks toward and away from the television. Each
child was videotaped during the entire experimental session, and visual
attention to the television was later coded from the videotape.
After the child was seated in the room, the experimenter obtained his or
her verbal assent to participate in the study. Once the child agreed to
participate, the experimenter explained the procedure to the child, empha-
sizing that watching the television program was the primary task and
responding to the probe was the secondary task. It was explained to the
participants that they would hear sounds that they should turn off as
quickly as possible by pressing the space bar on a computer keyboard
located on the table in front of them. Participants were instructed to keep
their dominant hand directly in front of the computer keyboard throughout
the entire television program. To help the child do this, the experimenter
placed a loose Velcro strap across the child’s wrist. After the procedure
was explained, the child was given an opportunity to practice. A computer
program presented five probes that were randomly spaced throughout a
2-min period. Each child pressed the space bar in response to every practice
probe. It was again emphasized to the child that watching and understand-
ing the program was the primary task, and each child was told that after the
program ended there would be some questions about the television show.
0.04). RT data
were analyzed after removal of all probe RTs that occurred during
a look away from the television ( n
0.96, SD
51, less than 5% of target
probes). The pattern of results is identical with these RTs included,
but analyses reported in this article exclude RTs to probes that
sounded during looks away from the television (Lorch & Castle,
1997).
RTs
Mean response times were analyzed in a repeated measures
analysis of variance (ANOVA), with centrality (central and inci-
dental) and probe position (2, 7, 12 s into sequence, and later
position) as within-subject variables. Follow-up linear trend anal-
yses tested the a priori prediction that RTs to probes would
increase as the time into a sequence of central content increased. In
addition, we tested whether RTs to probes would decrease as time
into a sequence of incidental content increased. Mean RTs as a
function of centrality and probe position are depicted in Figure 1.
As hypothesized, a significant interaction was observed between
centrality and probe position, F (3, 171) 3.32, p .022. Linear
1210
LORCH, MILICH, ASTRIN, AND BERTHIAUME
850
800
750
700
Cent r al
Incidental
650
600
550
2
7
12
Later
Probe Position (Seconds into Sequence)
Figure 1. Mean probe response times (RTs) as a function of time into central and incidental sequences for
Experiment 1.
trend analyses indicated that RTs to probes occurring during
central sequences increased as time into the sequences increased,
F (1, 57) 4.30, p .05 (effect size r .26), and RTs to probes
occurring during incidental sequences decreased as time into the
sequences increased, F (1, 57) 6.16, p .05 ( r .31).
Follow-up analyses controlling family-wise error rate indicated
that the mean RT in the central, 2-s position was significantly
shorter than the central, later position RT, t (58) 2.36, p .05
( r .30). The incidental, 2-s position RT was significantly slower
than the incidental, later position RT, t (58) 2.29, p .05 ( r
.29). Unexpectedly, children were significantly slower in respond-
ing to probes that sounded during incidental sequences ( M
612.73) than to probes during central sequences ( M 572.15),
F (1, 57) 9.84, p .004 ( r .38). No main effect was observed
for probe position ( F 1, p .05).
cally, one might expect that recall performance would be related to
differing levels of engagement. Each cued recall question tested
information that was presented during or close to a unit that
contained a probe. Mean RTs were computed for probes corre-
sponding to questions that each child answered correctly and
questions answered incorrectly. Regardless of centrality, chil-
dren’s RTs corresponding to questions they answered correctly
tended to be longer ( M 601.77) than their RTs corresponding to
questions they answered incorrectly ( M 587.83), F (1, 48)
3.71, p .06 ( r .27). This suggests an association between
online cognitive engagement with the televised story and later
recall of specific material.
As predicted, 9- to 11-year-old children’s cognitive engagement
was related to the continuity of central or incidental content.
Children’s probe RTs indicated that the longer a sequence of
central story content continued, the more engaged children be-
came, and that as a sequence of incidental content continued, they
became less engaged. Children’s increased engagement was also
related to better performance on story comprehension measures.
Further discussion of the results from all three experiments is
reserved for the general discussion.
Cued Recall Performance
We conducted a t test on cued recall scores to evaluate differ-
ences between performance on central and incidental content.
Performance on the cued recall questions was significantly better
for those questions tapping central content ( M 0.85, SD 0.11)
than for those tapping incidental content ( M 0.61, SD 0.18),
t (57) 12.05, p .001 ( r .84). This result replicates the pattern
observed in numerous previous studies of children’s comprehen-
sion of both televised and written stories (e.g., Lorch, Bellack, &
Augsbach, 1987; van den Broek, 1989; van den Broek et al., 1996).
A further question concerns the possibility of a relation between
cued recall performance and RTs to secondary probes. If children’s
cognitive engagement with the televised story varies systemati-
Experiment 2
Experiment 1 examined cognitive engagement in children ages
9 to 11 years, but an unanswered question is whether younger
school-aged children would also use the causal structure of a
televised story to guide their engagement. Memory for story events
among preschoolers and young school-age children is influenced
by characteristics of the story’s causal structure, but the impact of
92861850.001.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin