ingold_2007_Earth, sky, wind, and weather.pdf

(331 KB) Pobierz
Earth, sky, wind, and weather
Earth, sky, wind, and weather
T
im
I
ngold
University of Aberdeen
This paper seeks to understand what it means to live ‘in the open’. It begins with an account of
experiments that test whether children have acquired a scientifically correct understanding of the
shape of the earth, according to which people live all around on the outside of a solid sphere. This
understanding cannot accommodate the phenomenon of the sky, in relation to which the earth can
appear only as the ground of human habitation. James Gibson’s ecological approach to perception
offers a possible alternative, depicting earth and sky as complementary hemispheres. Yet for Gibson,
this earth-sky can be inhabited only insofar as it is furnished with objects. To that extent, it ceases to
be open. Drawing on elements of Merleau-Ponty’s phenomenology, it is argued that in the open
world persons and things relate not as closed forms but by virtue of their common immersion in
the generative fluxes of the medium – in wind and weather. Fundamental to life is the process of
respiration, by which organisms continually disrupt any boundary between earth and sky, binding
substance and medium together in forging their own growth and movement. Thus to inhabit the
open is not to be stranded on the outer surface of the earth but to be caught up in the
transformations of the weather-world.
We all know what it feels like to be out in the open air on a windy day. Yet once we try
to pin it down within established categories and conventions of thought, no experience
could be more elusive. What is the open air? Does it circulate in the sky or the
atmosphere? Are these the same or different? If the atmosphere surrounds our planet,
and the sky arches above our heads, then in what shape or form can the earth exist in
relation to the sky? And if we are out in the open world of earth and sky, how can we
simultaneously be in the wind? How, in other words, can we inhabit the open? If we can
do so only by containing it, then how can the wind still blow? In what follows I seek to
establish what it means to be ‘in the open’. Instead of thinking of the inhabited world as
composed of mutually exclusive hemispheres of sky and earth, separated by the
ground, we need to attend, as I shall show, to the fluxes of wind and weather. To feel the
wind is not to make external, tactile contact with our surroundings but to mingle with
them. In this mingling, as we live and breathe, the wind, light, and moisture of the sky
bind with the substances of the earth in the continual forging of a way through the
tangle of life-lines that comprise the land.
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.) ,S
19
-S
38
© Royal Anthropological Institute 2007
S20
Tim Ingold
To reach this conclusion I shall proceed in four stages. I begin with what is supposed
to be the objective, scientific account of the shape of the earth, an account that cannot
readily accommodate the phenomenon of the sky. For in relation to the sky, the earth
can exist only as a ground of habitation. Yet as I go on to show, a ground populated
solely by people and objects, and a sky that is empty but for birds and clouds, can exist
only within a simulacrum of the world, modelled in an interior space. The third stage
of the argument is to show that in the open world, beings relate not as closed, objective
forms but by virtue of their common immersion in the fluxes of the medium. The
process of respiration, by which air is taken in by organisms from the medium and in
turn surrendered to it, is fundamental to all life. Thus, finally, to inhabit the open is to
dwell within a weather-world in which every being is destined to combine wind, rain,
sunshine, and earth in the continuation of its own existence.
How to draw the sky
There is currently some controversy in the fields of cognitive and developmental
psychology concerning how children learn the shape of the earth. A number of studies
suggest that a correct understanding of the earth, as a solid sphere surrounded by space,
challenges fundamental presuppositions that children everywhere, regardless of cul-
tural background, initially bring to their reasoning about the world. These presuppo-
sitions are, firstly, that the ground is flat, and, secondly, that unless supported, things
fall. To grasp such a counter-intuitive understanding that the earth is round like a ball
and that people can live anywhere on its surface without falling off calls, it is argued, for
nothing less than a complete conceptual restructuring in the child’s mind, comparable
to a paradigm shift in the history of science. Experimenting with schoolchildren aged
between
and
11
).
But this research is not without its critics. They argue that the problems of recon-
ciliation that many children undoubtedly faced in these experiments have less to do
with their own intuitions or ‘naïve theories’ about the world than with the demands of
an experimental situation in which they are called upon to justify what they had said or
drawn in response to previous questioning. In reality, these critics claim, children do
not begin with any beliefs, intuitions or theories about the shape of the earth, but rather
set out with an open mind. Their knowledge is then acquired piecemeal, in loosely
connected fragments, through participation in a social and cultural environment that
is scaffolded by knowledgeable adults such as teachers, but also by artefacts such as the
ubiquitous globes of school classrooms. Since there is no initial conceptual barrier to be
overcome, and given adequate scaffolding, children have little difficulty in acquiring a
‘scientific’ picture of the earth. Indeed, experiments involving children having to
choose between ready-made pictures – rather than drawing them themselves or having
to respond to interrogation – seem to show little difference in understanding between
younger and older children, or even between children and adults (Nobes, Martin &
Panagiotaki
1992
; see Fig.
1
).
I do not intend to take a stance in this debate. It is but one version of a long-
standing argument in psychology about whether knowledge acquisition is strongly
2005
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.) ,S
19
-S
38
© Royal Anthropological Institute 2007
years, researchers claim to have identified a developmental sequence
in thinking about the earth, running from an initial mental model of an earth that is flat
like a pancake to a final model of a spherical earth, by way of various intermediate
models in which children attempt to reconcile their initial presuppositions with infor-
mation supplied by their teachers, or gleaned from books and other sources (Vosniadou
1994
6
; Vosniadou & Brewer
Tim Ingold S21
Figure 1. Mental models of the Earth. Reproduced from Vosniadou & Brewer (1992: 549) with the
kind permission of Elsevier.
constrained by innate mental structures or more fundamentally dependent on socio-
cultural contexts of learning. It is what the two sides have in common that interests
me. For neither is in any doubt that there exists a ‘scientifically correct’ account of the
shape of the earth, against which any alternatives may be judged more or less erro-
neous. 1 More remarkably, both sides seem to agree that where there is an earth, there
must also be a sky. How, then, might we render a ‘scientifically correct’ account of the
nature and shape of the sky? Let me present two examples, taken from studies rep-
resenting opposed positions in the debate outlined above, of what is taken to be the
‘correct’ view. In the first example,
6
1992
:
557
,
see Fig.
2
A).
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.) ,S
38
© Royal Anthropological Institute 2007
19
-S
-year-old Ethan tells the experimenter – in
response to her questions – that the earth has the shape of a ball, that to see it one
should look downwards, and that it is completely surrounded by space. The experi-
menter then asks Ethan to draw a picture of the earth, and he obliges with a rough
circle, within which he draws the outlines of what look like continents. ‘Now’, com-
mands the experimenter, ‘draw the sky’. Ethan is perplexed. ‘The sky has no shape’, he
protests, ‘you mean space’. Nevertheless, draw the sky he must, so he proceeds to
describe a ring around the circle depicting the earth (Vosniadou & Brewer
616672162.001.png
S22
Tim Ingold
Figure 2. A: Ethan’s drawing of the spherical earth surrounded by the ‘sky’. B: Darcy’s drawing of
the sky, the ground (with houses) and the spherical earth. Reproduced from Vosniadou & Brewer
(1992: 558) with the kind permission of Elsevier.
In the second example the experimenters prepared a set of picture-cards, each of
which showed the earth, people, and sky in one of sixteen possible combinations of
the following alternatives: earth a solid sphere, flattened sphere, hollow sphere, or
disc; people all around or only on top; sky all around or only on top. Participants, who
included both children (aged
5
-
10
). Some two-thirds of the participants in the study
selected, as their first choice, the combination of solid sphere with both people and
sky all around. On the card depicting this combination, the earth figures as a greeny-
brown ball, with rigid, Lego-like people standing around its circumference and set
against a light blue background flecked with fluffy white patches resembling clouds
(Fig.
2005
:
52
-
4
3
). The selection of this card by the majority of participants, according to the
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.) ,S
19
-S
38
© Royal Anthropological Institute 2007
) and adults, were individually asked first to select
the card they thought looked most like the real earth and then to repeat the procedure
with all the others so as to yield a ranking from ‘most’ to ‘least like the Earth’ (Nobes,
Martin & Panagiotaki
616672162.002.png
Tim Ingold S23
Figure 3. Spherical earth with people and sky around. Reproduced from Nobes et al. (2005: 54) with
the kind permission of the British Psychological Society.
). The picture is, however, strangely paradoxical. On the one hand, it depicts
people distributed around the outer surface of a solidly spherical earth, but, on the
other hand, it depicts the sky in a form that would be apparent only to someone lying
on his or her back on the earth’s surface, gazing upwards! The perspective that leads
us to recognize the earth as a ball is not one that could possibly yield an image of a
blue sky with scattered clouds.
The perspectival double-take involved in the attempt to combine the spherical earth
and the sky in the same picture confused participants in the first experiment, as much
as in the second. Ethan, as we have seen, took the experimenter to mean not sky but
space, and the gesture he made to signal his understanding that space is all around left
its trace in the outer ring of his drawing. But the reaction of another participant in this
study,
:
55
-
7
-year-old Darcy, is even more revealing. Responding to the experimenter’s
request, Darcy has drawn a round earth, and has added the moon and some stars. The
experimenter then asks Darcy – as she had asked Ethan – to draw the sky. Like Ethan,
Darcy is thrown by this. ‘It’s icky’, she says. Her solution, however, is to sketch some
roughly horizontal lines, looking much like a cloud-base, near the top of the paper, and
above her drawing of the earth, moon, and stars. When the experimenter asks where
people live, Darcy draws a house whose base lies along the lower border of the paper.
The experimenter asks again, and Darcy draws another house. On the third request,
Darcy eventually gives in to the experimenter’s implicit demands, rubs out one of her
houses, and draws a stick figure upon her round earth (Fig.
9
B). But this only sparks
off a further bout of interrogation. ‘This house is on the earth isn’t it?’, says the
experimenter, pointing to the sketch of the house that remains after the other was
2
38
© Royal Anthropological Institute 2007
19
-S
authors of this study, ‘indicated a scientific understanding of the Earth’ (Nobes et al.
2005
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.) ,S
616672162.003.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin