BORDERLINE PERSONALITY TRAITS AND INTIMATE PARTNER AGGRESSION AN INTERNATIONAL MULTISITE, CROSS-GENDER ANALYSIS.pdf

(144 KB) Pobierz
pwqu_437_LR
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32 (2008), 290–302. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA.
Copyright C
2008 Division 35, American Psychological Association. 0361-6843/08
BORDERLINE PERSONALITY TRAITS AND INTIMATE
PARTNER AGGRESSION: AN INTERNATIONAL MULTISITE,
CROSS-GENDER ANALYSIS
Denise A. Hines
ClarkUniversity
Although research has consistently shown that men and women use intimate partner aggression (IPA) at approximately
equal rates, there is little empirical research on whether the predictors of IPA are the same for men and women. The
current study investigated whether borderline personality (BP) differentially predicted the use of IPA for men and
women across 67 university sites from around the world. Results showed that BP predicted several forms of IPA and
that gender did not moderate the association between IPA and BP. Although the strength of the association between
IPA and BP significantly differed across sites, it was in the predicted direction for the overwhelming majority of sites.
Implications of these results and suggestions for future research are discussed.
Intimate partner aggression (IPA) is typically considered to
consist of physical, emotional, and/or sexual aggression—
any one of these in and of themselves can constitute IPA,
as can any combination of two or all three. Since the ad-
vent of research into IPA in the early 1970s, there has been
evidence that both women and men use physical and psy-
chological IPA at rates that are approximately equal (e.g.,
Gelles, 1974), whereas women’s use of sexual IPA is typi-
cally less thanmen’s (e.g., Cantos, Neidig, &O’Leary, 1994;
Hines & Saudino, 2003; Swan & Snow, 2002). Moreover,
several studies have shown that women and men initiate
physical IPA at equal rates and that, in 25% of violent re-
lationships, only the women had been physically aggressive
within the previous year. In 25% of violent relationships,
only the man had been physically aggressive within the pre-
vious year, and in 50% of violent relationships, both part-
ners were physically aggressive (Carrado, George, Loxam,
Jones, & Templar, 1996; Demaris, 1992; Hines & Saudino,
2003; Morse, 1995; Stets & Straus, 1990). The equivalent
rates of physical IPA by men and women has recently been
confirmed by a meta-analysis of dozens of studies of IPA
over a 30-year period (Archer, 2000) and is further con-
firmed by the dramatic rise in women’s arrests and pros-
ecution for domestic violence in states across the nation
since the inception of mandatory arrest policies (Buzawa
& Buzawa, 2002). Although there is an abundance of re-
search showing that men and women use approximately
equal rates of physical IPA, there is considerably less re-
search on whether the prediction of IPA is equivalent for
men and women.
IPAandBorderlinePersonality
Few researchers have investigated whether there are gen-
der differences in the extent to which personality dysfunc-
tion predicts various forms of IPA, even though there is a
growing body of research showing that criminal behavior
and physical IPA perpetration are manifestations of per-
sonality traits (e.g., Krueger, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2000) and
dysfunction (e.g., Holtzworth-Munroe, Meehan, Herron,
Rehman, & Stuart, 2000) in men. One personality dysfunc-
tion that is related to the use of IPA in men, and may be for
women, is borderline personality (BP). The termBP will be
used here to refer to traits of Borderline Personality Disor-
der (BPD) that do not necessarily meet diagnostic criteria.
BP is somewhat synonymous with what Dutton (1995) calls
“borderline personality organization,” which is a less severe
form of BPD and is exhibited by 11–15% of the population
(in comparison to 1.5–4%who exhibit BPD). Because BPD
is three times more common among women than among
men (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), it is likely
that BP is also more common among women, and it is,
Denise A. Hines, Department of Psychology, Clark University.
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental
Health grant T32MH15161 and by the University of New Hamp-
shire. This article is part of the International Dating Violence
Study. Other papers from that study can be downloaded from
http://pubpages.unh.edu/
290
mas2.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Denise A.
Hines, Clark University, Department of Psychology, 950 Main
St., Worcester, MA 01610. E-mail: dhines@clarku.edu
BorderlinePersonalityandAggression
291
therefore, a good candidate for being associated with IPA
in women.
According to Dutton (1995), the essential characteris-
tics of BP organization, from most important to least, are
“proclivity for intense, unstable interpersonal relationships
characterized by intermittent undermining of the signifi-
cant other, manipulation, and masked dependency; an un-
stable sense of self with intolerance of being alone and
abandonment anxiety; and intense anger, demandingness,
and impulsivity, usually tied to substance abuse or promis-
cuity” (p. 570). People with BP organization experience re-
lationships in which their emotional needs go unmet, and
they do not have the skills to assert their needs in a healthy
way. Frustrations can increase when they do not get what
they want or need in their intimate relationship. When they
perceive a possible loss of the relationship, people with BP
organization, who have an intense fear of abandonment and
loneliness, may experience intense anger or rage. Because
people with BP organization do not have the requisite skills
to appropriately handle their anger, they are at a high likeli-
hood of physically lashing out. In addition, because people
with BP organization see their significant other as either all
good or all bad, at this stage they tend to devalue their part-
ners, further increasing the likelihood of IPA. In an effort
to keep their partners from abandoning them, they may
engage in further manipulative and controlling behaviors
to keep them from leaving the relationship. Thus, people
with BP organization tend to have very intense, unstable
relationships and identities (Dutton, 1995).
Studies of men in batterer treatment programs support
the notion that BP traits are a strong predictor of their use
of physical IPA (e.g., Dutton, 1995; Dutton & Browning,
1988; Dutton, Starzomski, & Ryan, 1996; Hart, Dutton, &
Newlove, 1993; Hastings &Hamberger, 1988; Holtzworth-
Munroe, Bates, Smutzler, & Sandin, 1997; Mauricio, Tein,
& Lopez, 2007; Oldham et al., 1985) and psychological
IPA (Dutton & Starzomski, 1993; Mauricio et al., 2007;
Oldham et al., 1985). Their use of IPA is usually impul-
sive and a function of their mood rather than any external
stimulus. Men suffering from BP organization tend to be
emotionally volatile. They are angry, jealous, and depressed
men, who tend to use IPA when there is some sort of per-
ceived “socioemotional distance” between them and their
female partners (Dutton, 1995).
Themajority of this research has been conducted on clin-
ical samples of male batterers, and we do not know whether
these findings would generalize to women. However, pre-
liminary evidence suggests that some women who use IPA
might be suffering from BP. For example, Margolies and
Leeder (1995) provided a clinical description of lesbian
batterers in their intervention program. This description
mirrored symptoms of BP. The women in this study “ex-
hibited a striking propensity for dichotomous thinking and
feeling. They were prone to extreme views and hyperbole,
dividing the world into black and white” (p. 144). They
were overly dependent on their partners for both attention
and emotional support, and they often tried to emotionally
merge with their partners. They used violence when they
felt that their partners were becoming emotionally distant
or when there was a threatened physical separation. They
viewed these actions as abandonment, and they evidenced
a chronic fear of abandonment. Thus, violence was used to
maintain a connection to their partners.
Stuart, Moore, Gordon, Ramsey, and Kahler (2006), in
a comparison of women arrested for IPA and mandated
into treatment with women from the general population,
found that the odds of women arrested for IPA having BPD
were 20.3 times greater than women in the general popu-
lation. Comparison studies of men and women mandated
into treatment for IPA also show evidence that women who
perpetrate IPA evidence BP, perhaps even at rates that are
higher than those of men. For example, the female batter-
ers in Henning, Jones, and Holdford’s (2003) study were
significantly more likely to evidence BP patterns than the
male batterers (11.6% versus 2.8%).
Although there is evidence among clinical samples that
women with BP might be at greater risk of using IPA,
it is unknown whether these results would generalize to
nonclinical samples. The associations between BP and IPA
in nonclinical samples are important to consider because
most IPA never comes to the attention of authorities, yet
is still detrimental to the mental and physical health of
its victims (Straus & Gelles, 1990). In addition, although
BP predicts both physical and psychological IPA by men
and physical IPA by women, it is unknown whether these
associations would generalize to sexual IPA by both genders
or psychological IPA by women.
Moreover, IPA affects men and women in every region
of the world (World Health Organization, 2005; Straus &
the International Dating Violence Research Consortium,
2004), and studies show that BPD is evident all over the
world and is one of the most common types of personal-
ity disorders worldwide (Loranger et al., 1994). However,
there is little research demonstrating a link between BP
and IPA in non-Western cultures and countries other than
the United States. This possible link in other cultures and
countries is important to investigate because it could point
toward either similar or different prevention and interven-
tion techniques in other cultures. Moreover, if there is a
difference among cultures in the association between BP
and IPA, possible reasons for these differences should be
explored. For example, it is possible that the association
between BP and IPA is stronger in cultures in which IPA is
not culturally accepted, but weaker in cultures where IPA
is more common and acceptable.
The purpose of the current study was to evaluate:
(1) Whether BP is a significant predictor of physi-
cal, psychological, and sexual IPA among men and
women in a nonclinical population. Given the pre-
vious research establishing a positive association be-
tween physical and psychological IPA and BP among
292
H INES
men and women in clinical samples (e.g., Dutton &
Starzomski, 1993; Stuart et al., 2006), it is expected
that this association will also generalize to nonclinical
samples.
(2) Whether there are gender differences in the pre-
diction of physical, psychological, and sexual IPA
from BP. Previous research among male and fe-
male batterers who were arrested for IPA (Hen-
ning et al., 2003) suggests that BP may be more
common among female batterers than male bat-
terers. However, other empirical research of men
and women in community, nationally representa-
tive, clinical, and birth cohort samples (e.g., Busch
& Rosenberg, 2004; Capaldi & Owen, 2001; Carney
& Buttell, 2004; Giordano, Millhollin, Cernkovich,
Pugh, & Rudolph, 1999; Henning et al., 2003; Mag-
dol,Moffitt, Caspi, &Silva, 1998;Medeiros &Straus,
2006) suggests that there is gender symmetry in the
variables that predict IPA, including various person-
ality traits and dysfunctions. Therefore, it is hypoth-
esized that the association between BP and IPA will
be similar for men and women.
(3) Whether these associations hold constant across 67
sites from around the world. Because there has been
little to no cross-cultural research on the associa-
tion between BP and IPA, no specific hypotheses
were made concerning whether these associations
would be consistent across sites. However, if differ-
ences are found, reasons for such differences were
planned.
relationships and that it would include sensitive questions
concerning attitudes, beliefs, and experiences in a relation-
ship, including questions on sexual behavior. They were
guaranteed anonymity, and they were told that the session
would take about an hour. Students completed the ques-
tionnaire at their own pace and deposited the completed
(or if they chose, blank) questionnaire in a box and left the
room when they finished. A debriefing form was provided
as they turned in their questionnaire. It explained the study
in more detail and provided names and telephone numbers
of local mental health services and community resources,
such as services for battered women.
The percentage of students who chose to participate and
deposited a completed questionnaire ranged from 42% to
100%, with most participation rates ranging from 85% to
95%. A detailed description of the study, including the
questionnaires and all other key documents, is available on
the study website: http://pubpages.unh.edu/
3,152) were eliminated from the analy-
ses. Because of questionable data from the Iranian sample
that could not adequately be explained, the Iranian site was
eliminated from the analysis ( n
=
METHOD
98). This procedure left a
sample of 14,154 students (4,054 men and 10,100 women).
A list of the sites involved is given in Table 5.
Demographic characteristics of the sample are shown in
Table 1. These characteristics are presented for the sample
overall and for each gender, and the sites that represented
the high and low scores for each variable are also presented.
As shown for the men, sample sizes ranged from 4 men at
the Calcutta, India site to 163 men at the Swedish site. The
mean length of relationship for the male sample was 12.70
months, close to 70% of the relationships were sexual, and
95.8% of the men were involved in heterosexual intimate
relationships. For women, the sample sizes ranged from
20 at the Calcutta, India site to 511 at the Swedish site.
Women reported an average relationship length of 15.08
months, sex was part of the relationship in 73.3% of the
cases, and 98% of the women were involved in heterosexual
relationships.
=
Participants
The data for this article were from the International Dat-
ing Violence Study (IDVS). The IDVS was conducted by
members of a consortium of researchers at universities in
various regions of the world. Consortium members volun-
teered to become part of the study after hearing about the
study at international family violence conferences, because
of previous collaborations with the principal investigator, or
through word of mouth. The questionnaires were usually
administered in classes taught by members of the consor-
tium and in other classes for which they could make ar-
rangements. Thus, it was a convenience sample. Almost all
of the classes were introductory-level psychology, sociology,
and criminal justice studies courses; thus, the majority of
the sample consisted of female college students. The data
presented in the current analysis are from 67 university
sites around the world.
Questionnaires were distributed at the beginning of the
class period. The purpose of the study and that participa-
tion was entirely voluntary was explained orally and was also
written on the cover page of the questionnaire. The students
were informed that the questionnaire was about dating
Measures
There was a core questionnaire that each member of
the IDVS Research Consortium translated. All consortium
members agreed to back-translate to maintain conceptual
mas2.
The completed questionnaires were examined for ques-
tionable response patterns, such as reporting an injury from
dating violence but not reporting an assault as having oc-
curred, or reporting an attack on a partner with a knife or
gun 10 or more times in the past year. About 7.5% of the
original 15,321 cases were identified as questionable and
were removed from the sample, leaving 14,252 students. In
addition, only students who were currently or recently (i.e.,
within the previous year) involved in a romantic relationship
were instructed to complete the measure of dating aggres-
sion; students who did not complete the measure of dating
aggression ( n
BorderlinePersonalityandAggression
293
Table 1
Demographics
Total
Men
Women
t or
χ
2
n
14,154
4,054
10,100
High
674
163
511
(Gavle, Sweden)
(Gavle, Sweden)
(Gavle, Sweden)
Low
24
4
20
(Calcutta, India)
(Calcutta, India)
(Calcutta, India)
Individual Demographics
Age (M, SD)
23.12, 6.28
23.01, 5.56
23.16, 6.55
1.41
High
39.06, 9.71
38.09, 10.04
39.98, 9.14
(Liege, Belgium)
(German-speaking Switzerland)
(Liege, Belgium)
Low
18.65, 1.36
18.86, 1.60
18.45, 1.04
(St. Petersburg 2, Russia)
(St. Petersburg 2, Russia)
(St. Petersburg 2, Russia)
Social Desirability (M, SD)
2.62, 0.36
2.59, 0.35
2.63, 0.36
6.49 ∗∗∗
High
2.91, 0.42
2.88
2.94, 0.40
(Merida, Venezuela)
(Cluj-Napcoa, Romania,
(Merida, Venezuela)
0.39; Northern
Mexico, SD
=
0.35;
Merida, Venezuela,
SD
=
=
0.46)
Low
2.39, 0.38
2.34, 0.38
2.39, 0.36
(Mito, Japan)
(Washington, DC, USA)
(Mito, Japan)
Borderline Personality (M, SD)
1.98, 0.49
2.00, 0.45
1.97, 0.50
3.96 ∗∗∗
High
2.42, 0.44
2.29, 0.44
2.48, 0.44
(Taiwan)
(Beijing, China)
(Taiwan)
Low
1.58, 0.41
1.52, 0.34
1.59, 0.42
(Leiden, Netherlands)
(Leiden, Netherlands)
(Leiden, Netherlands)
Relationship Demographics
Relationship Length
14.40, 8.92
12.70, 9.01
15.08, 8.80
14.31 ∗∗∗
in months (M, SD)
High
19.46, 7.27
19.13, 8.01
19.90, 7.01
(Gavle, Sweden)
(Leiden, Netherlands)
(Gavle, Sweden)
Low
6.21, 6.71
6.04, 5.80
6.38, 7.57
(St. Petersburg 2, Russia)
(St. Petersburg 2, Russia)
(St. Petersburg 2, Russia)
% Sex in Relationship
72.1%
69.2%
73.3%
24.30 ∗∗∗
High
97.8%
98.2%
99.0%
(Gavle, Sweden &
(Quebec 2, Canada)
(Freiburg, Germany)
Freiburg, Germany)
Low
11.8%
5.9%
14.5%
(Varsanyi, Hungary)
(Varsanyi, Hungary)
(Varsanyi, Hungary)
% Heterosexual Relationships
97.4%
95.8%
98.0%
55.47 ∗∗∗
High
99.6%
100%
100%
(Cluj-Napcoa, Romania)
(10 sites)
(9 sites)
Low
92.7%
68.2%
92.4%
(Jackson, MS, USA)
(Jackson, MS, USA)
(Washington, DC, USA)
Intimate Partner Aggression
% Any Physical Aggression
29.0%
23.9%
31.0%
70.14 ∗∗∗
High
49.6%
54.7%
54.8%
(New York, NY, USA)
(New York, NY, USA)
(Vladivostok, Russia)
Low
15.4%
9.4%
11.6%
(Houston, TX, USA)
(Hamilton, Canada)
(Houston, TX, USA)
% Severe Physical Aggression
10.4%
8.0%
11.3%
34.95 ∗∗∗
High
24.4%
28.6%
25.0%
(New York, NY, USA)
(Capetown, South Africa)
(Crete 2, Greece)
Low
1.8%
0%
2.0%
(Gavle, Sweden)
(Calcutta, India; Singapore;
(Amsterdam, Netherlands)
Hamilton, Canada; Malta)
(continued)
SD
270617738.001.png 270617738.002.png
294
H INES
Table 1 (continued)
Total
Men
Women
t or
χ
2
Types of Physical Aggression
0.69, 1.47
0.54, 1.29
0.76, 1.53
8.69 ∗∗∗
(M, SD)
High
1.57, 2.22
2.29, 3.95
1.65, 2.32
(New York, NY, USA)
(Capetown, South Africa)
(New York, NY, USA)
Low
0.33
0.13, 0.42
0.23, 0.78
(Portugal, SD
=
.98;
(Hamilton, Canada)
(Houston, TX, USA)
Malta, SD
=
.82)
% Any Psychological Aggression
72.6%
65.7%
75.3%
133.96 ∗∗∗
High
91.0%
100%
90.9%
(Washington, DC, USA)
(Calcultta, India)
(Washington, DC, USA)
Low
45.7%
36.7%
42.9%
(St. Petersburg 1, Russia)
(Shanghai)
(St. Petersburg 1, Russia)
% Severe Psychological
24.0%
20.9%
25.3%
30.47 ∗∗∗
Aggression
High
57.7%
50.8%
60.3%
(Hong Kong 3)
(Hong Kong 1)
(Hong Kong 3)
Low
8.3%
0%
7.6%
(Gavle, Sweden)
(Winnipeg, Canada)
(Gavle, Sweden)
Types of Psychological
2.04, 1.82
1.72, 1.76
2.17, 1.82
13.55 ∗∗∗
Aggression (M, SD)
High
3.03, 1.98
2.92, 1.51
3.07, 1.98
(New York, NY, USA)
(Washington, DC, USA)
(New York, NY, USA)
Low
1.20, 1.60
0.88, 1.58
1.25, 1.72
(St. Petersburg 1, Russia)
(Shanghai, China)
(St. Petersburg 1, Russia)
% Any Sexual Aggression
23.5%
29.3%
21.2%
105.00 ∗∗∗
High
60.9%
65.3%
58.3%
(Merida, Venezuela)
(Merida, Venezuela)
(Merida, Venezuela)
Low
5.2%
4.6%
4.7%
(Leiden, Netherlands)
(Hong Kong 1)
(Leiden, Netherlands)
% Forced Sexual Aggression
1.7%
2.2%
1.5%
7.71 ∗∗
High
7.2%
14.3%
10.5%
(Louisiana, USA)
(Capetown, South Africa)
(Dar Salaam, Tanzania)
Low
0%
0%
0%
(7 sites)
(25 sites)
(15 sites)
Types of Sexual Aggression
0.34, 0.73
0.47, 0.89
0.29, 0.64
12.18 ∗∗∗
(M, SD)
High
0.86, 0.98
1.11, 1.15
0.93, 1.26
(Merida, Venezuela)
(New York, NY, USA)
(Dar Salaam, Tanzania)
Low
0.06, 0.25
0.07, 0.26
0.05, 0.21
(Leiden, Netherlands)
(Hong Kong 3)
(Leiden, Netherlands)
Note. Gender differences between the demographic variables were explored using chi-square or t tests, where appropriate. The Social Desirability
and Borderline Personality scales ranged from 1 to 4. The maximum number of types of physical aggression was 12; the maximum number of types of
psychological aggression was 8, and the maximum number of types of sexual aggression was 7.
∗∗ p
.01. ∗∗∗ p
<
.001.
equivalence (Straus, 1969) across the sites. This core ques-
tionnaire consisted of demographic items (e.g., gender,
gender of partner, whether sex was part of the relation-
ship, age of participant, parents’ education and income,
length of relationship), the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales
(CTS2; Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, &Sugarman, 1996),
and the Personal and Relationships Profile (PRP; Straus,
Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1999). In addition,
the consortium members added questions to measure vari-
ables that were uniquely important for their site or con-
structs that were needed to test a theory of particular in-
terest. For the current study, only the demographic infor-
mation and questions pertaining to the use of IPA, BP, and
social desirability were used.
IPA. The use of IPA was measured by the physical ag-
gression, psychological aggression, and sexual aggression
scales of the CTS2. For each participant, the number of
different types of physically aggressive, psychologically ag-
gressive, and sexually aggressive acts used in the previous
<
270617738.003.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin