Chapter01-11.pdf
(
383 KB
)
Pobierz
Chapter01-11
The Critical Period Hypothesis and Second Language Acquisition
7
1. The Critical Period Hypothesis and Second Language Acquisition
Introduction
MuchresearchonL2acquisitioniseitherimplicitlyorexplicitlyinformedbytheCritical
Period Hypothesis, that is, the idea that maturational changes enforce an asymmetry
between language acquisition early in life compared to later acquisition. Originally
formulatedbyPenfield&Robert(1959)andLenneberg(1967),(versionsof)theCritical
Period Hypothesis have widely been recruited to explain both the success of child L1
acquisition and the comparative failure of adult L2 acquisition. This chapter introduces
theconceptofcriticalperiodsinbehaviouralandcognitivedevelopmentanddiscussesits
application to language acquisition. The major part of this chapter deals with critical
periodsinthecontextofL2acquisition.Afterdiscussingthecharacteristicsofpotential
critical periods and advancing a working definition of a critical period (Section 1.3), I
reviewtheempiricalevidenceadducedinfavourofandagainsttheexistenceofbounded
agerelatedconstraintsonL2acquisition(Sections1.3.2to1.3.5).
This chapter aims to give a general overview of the various ways in which the
investigation of agerelated effects in L2 acquisition has been tackled. Some studies
representative of each way will be discussed and their respective merits and lacunae
outlined. On the basis of this review, I introduce the conceptual basics and rationale of
generative approaches to the study of L2 ultimate attainment. These approaches will be
discussedwithaparticularemphasisontheacquisitionofphenomenaattheinterfacesof
syntaxwithothergrammaticalmodules.Severalrepresentativestudieswillbediscussed
toillustratetheoreticalapproachestolimitationsonadultL2acquisitionthatwillinform
theempiricalstudyofthisthesis.
1.1. Critical Periods: Features and biological foundations
A critical period describes a maturationally constrained period of time in which the
interactionofextantneurophysiologicalarchitectureandrelevantexternalstimuliresults
inthefulldevelopmentofaparticularcapacity.
1
Hence,acriticalperiodrepresentsatime
of heightened sensitivity to environmental stimuli for acquiring essential aptitudes that
willnotdevelopatallornotfullydevelopifexposuretotherelevantinputoccursearlier
orlaterthaninthebiologicallydeterminedtimewindow.
1
In the literature on maturationally governed developmental processes, the terms sensitive or optimal
period are often used (for discussion, see Ruben, 1997). A sensitive period denotes a less categorically
circumscribedperiodofheightenedsensitivitythansuggestedbytheclassicconceptionofacriticalperiod.
Inthefollowing,Iwillretainuseofthetermcriticalperiodforexpositoryconvenience(seealsoEubank&
Gregg, 1999; Hyltenstam & Abrahamsson, 2003), acknowledging, however, that the time span and the
slopeoftheonsetsandoffsetsofcriticalperiods(seeFigure1.1)mightbevariableandsmearoutacross
phenomena.
8
Chapter 1
Generically, then, a critical period is preceded by a period of low sensitivity to a given
stimuli and the subsequent critical period encompasses an onset, a peak period and an
offset,asillustratedinFigure1.1.Itissucceededbythesubsequentflatteningoutofthe
ability to acquire a behavioural process in response to the particular stimuli. Typically,
theonsetofacriticalperiodisratherabrupt,whiletheoffsetismoregradual;however,
different geometries of critical periods can be envisaged and have been put forth (see,
e.g.,Bornstein,1989,andBirdsong,2005afordiscussion).
PEAK
S
E
N
S
I
T
I
V
I
T
Y
O
N
S
E
T
O
F
F
S
E
T
Figure 1.1.Geometry of prototypical critical period (adapted from Birdsong, 2005a).
Critical periods have been found to underlie the development of diverse neurosensory
capacities in biology (Knudsen, 2004). Critical periods conforming to the geometry in
Figure1.1havebeenobserved,forinstance,inthedevelopmentofvisioninferals(e.g.
Hubel & Wiesel, 1970), haptic perception in mice (Glazewski, Chen, Silva, & Fox,
1996),thematingsonginaspeciesofAfricanfrogs(Watson&Kelley,1992),auditory
orientation in the barn owl (Knudsen & Knudsen, 1990) and the song of various bird
species,e.g.zebrafinchesandsparrows(Marler,1990).
Perhaps the closest analogue to the development of human language, the
development of speciesspecific birdsong, has been demonstrated to be contingent on
exposure to the song of conspecifics in the first year of life of certain bird species.
Further,youngbirdsmustbeabletohearthemselvesduringthattimeinordertodevelop
normalsongpatterns(e.g.Brenowitz,Margoliash&Nordeen,1997).Iftheseconditions
arenot met,thebirds willdevelopabnormal songpatterns that willnotbequalitatively
affected by later exposure to conspecific song patterns. Experimentally, it has further
beenshownthattheemergenceandthecharacteristicsofbirdsongcanbemodulatedby
direct neurophysiological intervention (e.g. lesioning brain areas dedicated to song
learning)orhormonalmanipulation,whichsuggeststhattheonsetandoffsetofacritical
periodforbirdsongisneurallyfoundedandhormonallyregulated(ibid).
CRITICALPERIOD
The Critical Period Hypothesis and Second Language Acquisition
9
In the human development of hearing, recent studies on auditory perception and
speechcomprehensionofcongenitallydeafchildrenwhohavebeenfittedwithcochlear
implantsatvariousagessuggestthatagemediatesthesuccessfulacquisitionofauditory
discrimination skills. Cochlear implants are surgically implanted devices that transform
auditory signals into electrical pulses to stimulate the auditory nerves in individuals
suffering from severe to profound hearing loss in order to emulate environmental
auditory stimuli, in particular speech. Several studies finda nonlinear relation between
age of implantation and the development of auditory skills. They suggest that normal
speech recognition development is contingent upon auditory input during a constrained
critical period in the first few years of life (e.g. McConkey Robbins, Burton Koch,
Osberger,ZimmermanPhillips&KishonRabin,2004;Sharma,Dorman&Spahr,2002;
for critical discussion, see Harrison, Gordon & Mount, 2005). In view of these critical
periodsintheacquisitionofsensorimotorcapacitiesservinglanguagecomprehension,it
is natural to ask whether critical periods can also be found for the neurocognitive
representationsoflanguage.
1.2. The Critical Period Hypothesis and language acquisition
OriginallyformulatedbyPenfield&Roberts(1959)andfurtherelaboratedbyLenneberg
(1967), the idea that a constrained period of heightened neural sensitivity underlies
language development has become widely espoused in research on first language
acquisition to account for the contrast in speed, ease and success of normal child
language development and the failure in later language acquisition when no relevant
linguisticinputwasavailableduringearlychildhood.
Evidence to underscore the reality of critical periods for language acquisition is
furnished by (a) feral children (for review, e.g. Curtiss, 1988), (b) congenitally deaf
children acquiring sign language later in life (e.g. Mayberry & Lock, 2003) and (c)
differences in auditory discrimination abilities of nonnative phonemic contrasts in
childrenatdifferentages(e.g.Werker&Tees,2005).
Although a critical period is often ascribed to language acquisition in general,
there has been growing awareness that ‘language’ is too coarsegrained a concept to
whichtoattributematurationaleffectsin toto(e.g.Eubank&Gregg,1999;Long,1990;
Newport et al., 2001). Seeing that ‘language’ consists of multiple conceptually and
neurallydistinguishablesubcomponentssuchassyntax,semantics,phonology,etc.,each
of these components could either be subject to critical periods of potentially different
properties,scopesandtemporalcharacteristicsorbesparedfrommaturationalconstraints
altogether.
Support for a differentiated conceptualization of multiple critical periods in
languageacquisitioncanindeedbefoundinmanyofthestudiesdocumentinglinguistic
failureindelayedfirstlanguageacquisition.Forinstance,thewellknowncaseofGenie,
10
Chapter 1
whowascompletelydeprivedoflinguisticinputfromtheageof1;6to13years(Curtiss,
1988), illustrates dissociations between linguistic components. After rescue, Genie
showedquickdevelopmentofvocabularyandcommunicativeskills,whereashersyntax
and intonation remained rudimentary even after prolonged exposure (for further
examples,seePinker,1994:290296;Singleton&Ryan,2004:3160).
More recent studies furnish evidence that even subcomponents of linguistic
domains, such as syntax or phonology, can be differentially affected by maturational
constraints.Inrecentworkonlateacquiredsignlanguage,Mayberry&Lock(2003;see
also Mayberry & Lock, 2002) report on a comparative study of congenitally deaf
individuals who had no exposure to (signed) linguistic input prior to age six and were
testedmorethan11yearsafterthedelayedonsetoftuitionandusageofEnglishasadults.
Their linguistic knowledge was compared to three groups of adults who had received
spoken or signed English input or spoken nonEnglish (Urdu) input in early childhood
and had had comparable lengths of exposure to English. On several tasks, the
congenitally deafindividualswith noearly exposure to languagecorrectly judged some
syntacticviolations(ungrammaticalauxiliaryselection,ungrammaticalobjectplacement,
ungrammatical conjunction) but performed at chance on identifying ungrammatical
structures involving syntactic movement (passives, relative clauses). By contrast, the
other groups showed no such asymmetry in behaviour. For phonology, findings of a
similar nature indicating different age effects depending on the type of phonological
phenomena have been reported in the context of cochlear implantation by Harrison,
Gordon&Mount(2005),andinthecontextofnormalchilddevelopmentbyWerker&
Tees(2005).
In sum, the robust findings that humans with no early exposure to language
demonstrate persistent failure in late language acquisition suggest that critical periods
constrain language development. However, different domains of language are
differentially affected by early deprivation of input, with some showing better
recoverabilityupondelayedexposurethanothers.
1.3. The Critical Period Hypothesis and second language acquisition
1.3.1. Conceptualizing the Critical Period Hypothesis in L2 acquisition
Conceptualizing a critical period in the context of adult second language acquisition
requiressevererevisionstothewaycriticalperiodsarecommonlycharacterized(Eubank
&Gregg,1999).Incontrasttotheprototypicalcasesofcriticalperiods,wherenorelevant
external stimuli were available at critical moments, adult L2 acquisition by definition
occurs against the background of successful L1 acquisition during the critical period.
Hence,anadultL2learnercannotbesaidtohavemissedthecriticalperiodforlanguage
acquisitionoutright;atthesametime,acquiringtheL2indeedoccursoutsidethecritical
The Critical Period Hypothesis and Second Language Acquisition
11
period. Assuming, then, that L1 acquisition entails a particular neurocognitive
organization of linguistic knowledge, acquiring an L2 in adulthood requires changes to
the extant patterns established for the L1. Irrespective of the conceptualization of these
changes, such neurocognitive reorganization implicates much more limited changes in
neural architecture and cognitive processes compared to the largescale organization of
linguistic knowledge that underlies the transition from the child’s prelinguistic state to
its mature state (e.g. Lenneberg, 1967: 176). As a consequence, e.g. Eubank & Gregg
(1999)cautionthatthebehaviouralreflexesofcriticalperiodeffectsobserveableinadult
secondlanguageacquisitionshouldberelativelyminorinscope.
Againstthisbackground,criticalperiodeffectsalsoneedtobedifferentiatedfrom
ageeffectsinL2acquisitionthatarenotmaturationallybounded.LateL2acquisitionis
likelytobesusceptibletogradualeffectsofcognitiveagingthatbegininearlyadulthood.
Cognitive aging affects language performance in causing slower and less efficient
informationprocessingandreducedworkingmemorycapacities(e.g.Park,2000).Inand
ofthemselves,cognitiveeffectsofagingmayimplicatelesssuccessfuloutcomesinadult
L2 acquisition (e.g. Bialystok & Hakuta, 1999; Hakuta, Bialystok, & Wiley, 2003).
Crucially, these age effects should be continuous and show a linear relation to age as
opposed to the nonlinear relation of age and acquisition predicted by a critical period
(Figure1.1).
In addition, the componential organization of language potentially entails that
theremightbedifferentcriticalperiodsfordifferentlinguisticsubcomponents,withsome
subcomponentsshowingnocriticalperiodsandothersbeingsubjecttocriticalperiodsof
different onsets and offsets. In recent reviews of the literature on critical periods in L2
acquisition,Long(2005)andSingleton(2005)giveoverviewsofthevaryingendsofthe
offset that have been proposed for subparts of language. For phonetics and phonology,
theproposalsrangefromoneyear(e.g.Ruben,1997)totwelveyearsofage(e.g.Scovel,
1988),formorphosyntax,thesuggestionsextendfromshortlyafterbirth(Hyltenstam&
Abrahamsson,2003)tofifteenyearsofage(Long,1990;2005).Table1.1(adaptedfrom
Singleton,2005)presentsaschematicoverviewofsomeproposals.
Plik z chomika:
music12
Inne pliki z tego folderu:
Piosenki Dla Dzieci-Fasolki - Domowe Przedszkole.zip
(3186 KB)
Efekt Tomatisa.doc
(48 KB)
5032.pdf
(169 KB)
Age and Second Language Acquisition and Processing.pdf
(217 KB)
Chapter01-11.pdf
(383 KB)
Inne foldery tego chomika:
Pliki dostępne do 01.06.2025
Pliki dostępne do 19.01.2025
Ebooki
Galeria
książki
Zgłoś jeśli
naruszono regulamin