Es, French - Face-to-face Versus Internet.pdf

(59 KB) Pobierz
Resolving Conflicts Over Ethical Issues: Face-to-face Versus Internet Negotiations
Resolving Conflicts over Ethical
Issues: Face-to-face Versus Internet
Negotiations
Robert van Es
Warren French
Felix Stellmaszek
ABSTRACT. Is the Internet an appropriate medium to
use when attempting to resolve conflicts over ethical is-
sues in business? The research reported on in this paper
focuses on internet versus face-to-face negotiations as a
component of applied discourse ethics. Although internet
negotiation has serious restrictions, it also has specific
qualities. It enhances reflection and plays down emotion.
Important qualities when handling complex and delicate
ethical issues.
discussion. With this in mind Drake et al. (2000)
present a thought provoking argument that infor-
mation technology can go a long way to facilitate
moral dialogues in business. Grounding their argu-
ment on the theory of moral discourse presented by
Habermas, they list potential benefits of ethical
negotiation conducted over the Internet. Certainly,
in an era of globalized trade, when making time to
meet those with whom one has an ethical problem is
a begrudged use of a scarce resource, there is an
allure to Drake’s argument.
A derivative question relates to the values
underpinning an ethical argument. Habermas (1979)
has claimed the most successful moral dialogues will
result in a new position based on shared values
arising from the discourse. The assumption made is
that negotiators will discover (reorder) their values
during the course of the moral dialogue.
French et al. (2002) investigated negotiation ethics
as a component of applied discourse ethics. They
found that the expression of shared values is not
sufficient to resolve ethical conflicts. Resolutions of
conflicts over ethical issues are produced by a mutual
reframing process during constructivist negotiation
with little attention explicitly paid to shared values.
Schwartz (1996) presents evidence that people do
reorder priorities among their values, dependent on
the situations, which they face. He has found reor-
dering between situations to be most likely in the
form of shifting from one value to a compatible value.
Compatibility refers to values within the same cate-
gorical dimension and, to a lesser extent, to another
categorical dimension, which is close to the original
dimension under his graphical scheme. What
Schwartz did not investigate is whether people reor-
der their values within any one situation in the process
of moral dialogue or constructivist negotiation.
KEY WORDS: moral dialogue, negotiation ethics, face-
to-face negotiations, Internet negotiations, universalism,
benevolence
Negotiating ethics and reordering values
How much time do businesspeople have for
thoughtful resolutions to ethical problems? Meeting
face-to-face with a supplier, colleague or client to
resolve an ethical issue takes a block of time – the
span of which is usually not known ahead of the
Robert van Es is Lecturer in Organizational Philosophy at the
University of Amsterdam, and Consultant in Organizational
Culture and Ethics. His research area is negotiating ethics
across cultures.
Warren French is the I.W. Cousins Professor of Business Ethics
at the Terry School – University of Georgia. He also serves as
a visiting faculty member at the University of Lyon III where
he teaches business ethics. His research area is conflict reso-
lution through discourse ethics.
Felix Stellmaszek earned an MBA at the University of Georgia
and is a Cand.rer.pol. at the University of Erlangen-
N¨rnberg. He has worked as a consultant both for Porsche
and for Accenture.
Journal of Business Ethics 53: 165–172, 2004.
2004 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.
590225941.001.png 590225941.002.png
166
Robert van Es et al.
Grounded in the work of Drake, French, Haber-
mas and Schwartz the following research proposi-
tions are posited. While Drake et al. (2000) pose an
interesting theory about the efficiency and effec-
tiveness of moral dialogue over the Internet; does
further data support their theory? Our first proposi-
tion is ‘‘Negotiations concerning a business issue
with ethical ramifications, conducted over the In-
ternet, will produce a greater percentage of successful
resolutions than those conducted face-to-face’’.
Past research by Van Es (1996) has found that
values are not brought up to great extent in face-to-
face business negotiations over an ethical issue. Will
the physical separation and asynchronous timing of
Internet negotiations lead to more disclosure of
values? Our second proposition is ‘‘There will be
more mentions of values in Internet negotiations
concerning a business issue with ethical ramifications
than in face to face negotiations’’.
Past research by French et al. (2001) has ques-
tioned whether shared values appear without guid-
ance in face-to-face business negotiations over
ethical issues. Will the physical separation and
asynchronous timing of Internet negotiations lead to
the recognition of more shared values upon which a
resolution can be based? Our third proposition is
‘‘Resolutions to conflict over a business issue with
ethical ramifications will be based more on com-
patible values rather than on shared values’’.
The argumentation used in these two sets of
negotiations was analyzed using Schwartz’s classifi-
cation for values as well as Brown’s (1996) classifi-
cation for ethical arguments. Brown’s basic model
contains four elements relating to ethical discourse:
1. Proposals: prescriptive statements that suggest
action,
2. Observations: descriptive statements that por-
tray situations,
3. Value Judgments: normative statements that
guide actions, and
4. Assumptions: reflective statements that express
views and attitudes.
Brown’s justification for his coding scheme comes
from his adaptation of the argumentation model
posited by Toulmin (1957). He adapts Toulmin’s
model to ethical arguments by substituting discourse
ethics terminology into Toulmin’s logical argu-
mentation process. This adaptation process is de-
scribed in detail in Brown’s (1990) earlier text.
Schwartz (1996), in turn, claims that a set of
universal values exists in cultures around the world.
He further claims that there are 10 such values,
grouped under four dimensions, which he labels self-
transcendence, self-enhancement, conservation and
openness to change (see Table I for a listing of these
values). The basis for his claim is a set of data
gathered from over 40 countries located on six
continents. The priorities assigned to these values,
however, differ between countries, within countries
and even within people. It is the degree to which
they shift within people that is of interest in this
research. To this end two judges jointly coded paper
copy transcripts of the 20 Internet negotiations that
had been conducted on a restricted access discussion
board. The same judges then coded the 20 audio-
taped, face-to-face negotiations. Disagreements over
coding were reconciled by referring back to the
detailed work of both Brown and Schwartz.
Research method
A business case with strong ethical ramifications was
created to investigate the three research propositions
(see Appendix 1). This case was analyzed by a large
set of subjects who made a binary choice – to either
keep a chemical plant open or shut it down. Then 40
subjects, split into 20 pairs based on their decisions
about the case, negotiated the case over the Internet.
Another 40 subjects, also assigned to 20 pairs,
negotiated the case face-to-face. Each of the sub-
jects, all U.S. citizens, possessed a university degree
and had been in the work place before returning to a
university setting for advanced business training.
Our samples are modest in numbers and show a
strong homogeneity; therefore the results of our
research are primarily to be regarded as good heu-
ristic insights.
Analysis of Internet Negotiations
The main values voiced in this particular case, using
Schwartz’s taxonomy, were: universalism (usually
expressed by those who initially wanted to shut the
factory down), and benevolence (usually expressed
Face-to-face Versus Internet Negotiations
167
TABLE I
Values underlying the ethical argumentation
TABLE II
Values expressed in negotiations regarding an ethical issue
Keep plant open Shut plant down
Successful
negotiations
Failed
negotiations
Self-transcendence a
Universalism 7
38
Internet negotiations
Shared values
Benevolence
32
6
4
3
Openness to change
Self Direction
Compatible values a
8
1
9
20
Disparate values
2
2
Stimulation
1
1
Face-to-face negotiations
Shared values
Conservation
Security
8
2
26
3
Compatible values
2
7
Tradition
6
4
Disparate values
1
0
Conformity
6
2
a Compatible values are restricted in this research to values
classified under the same dimension. The four dimensions
are listed in Table I.
Self-enhancement
Power
15
3
Achievement
0
0
Hedonism b
6
1
a 64 of the 80 subjects mentioned more than one value.
b Hedonism can be classified under the openness to change
dimension as well.
perceived responsibility. They were more open to
alternative resolutions if the employees’ economic
well-being was assured.
Fourteen of the 20 Internet negotiations resulted
in a mutually agreeable resolution (see Table II).
Two of these negotiations were conducted syn-
chronously, both resulting in a resolution. Syn-
chronous Internet negotiations contained more
(average 19) but shorter comments than did asyn-
chronous negotiations. The number of different
statements input by both parties into the asynchro-
nous Internet negotiations ranged from 6 to 12 with
a median average being 11. Again, all statements
were coded using Marvin Brown’s four-part cate-
gorization scheme (see Table III). Each negotiator’s
entire comment, irrespective of length, was consid-
ered as one statement. Any one statement was coded
to allow for more than one of the four designations.
The Internet negotiators reviewed previous
statements quite often and, consequently, reviewed
each other’s assumptions as well as the relatively few
observations that were made. This might be typical
for Internet based negotiations, since rereading
previous statements precludes the need to rehash
objective facts. In truth, negotiators using the In-
ternet may not have to be as well prepared as those
negotiating face-to-face, since they have time lags
between their comments to reflect on both past
statements and possible future tactics. Evidence for
this conjecture derives from the fact that early
statements were reread seven times on average,
by those who initially wanted to keep the plant
open) (see Table I). The difference in applying these
values often reduced to the span of those harmed:
(a) wide definition of non-maleficence for those
expressing a value of universalism (deonto-
logical argumentation about physical harm to
human life),
(b) narrow definition of non-maleficence for
those expressing limited benevolence (teleo-
logical argumentation about responsibility of
managers for employees/families).
There was a higher likelihood for a resolution when
both parties were open to new proposals. Addi-
tionally, the likelihood for a resolution increased
when alternatives were given at the beginning of the
negotiation. When this occurred, one or both parties
continued the rest of the negotiation without fear of
losing respect in the eyes of the other for abandoning
the original decision.
Generally, the negotiators who initially wanted to
shut the plant down were less flexible than those
who wanted to keep it open. Their prime value was
not to physically harm others. Those who wanted to
keep the plant open tended to consider only the
well-being of employees, for whom they had a
590225941.003.png
168
Robert van Es et al.
TABLE III
Profiles of ethical speech patterns by success of the negotiation
Proposal
average (%)
Observation
average (%)
Value judgment
average (%)
Assumption
average (%)
Total (%)
Successful internet negotiations
35.9
17.4
24.2
22.5
100.0
Successful face-to-face negotiations 37.4
16.4
21.6
24.6
100.0
Failed internet negotiations
27.7
15.6
28.9
27.8
100.0
Failed face-to-face negotiations
16.1
18.0
37.4
28.5
100.0
while latter statements were reread four times on
average.
Very few of the comments seemed to express
intense emotion. In contrast, a few of the face-to-
face negotiations were highly charged with emotion.
Many of the Internet negotiation statements ap-
peared to follow a logical progression. Some nego-
tiators even listed the arguments of the adversary and
addressed these points in sequential order. That
would be quite difficult in face-to-face negations if
there were numerous and/or complex points.
However, in one case where a participant tried to
control the Internet negotiation using sequential,
deductive logic and textbook negotiation tech-
niques, there was intransigence due to the other
party’s perception of being controlled and not being
an equal party to the resolution.
In each of the 20 Internet negotiations underlying
values were made clear. It appeared that negotiators
wanted to reveal their reasoning in order to justify
their decisions. Rarely was there a search, though,
for a resolution based on a shared value even though
shared values existed (see Table II). Also, fewer value
reordering than expected appeared in these negoti-
ations. Perhaps, this is because Schwartz’s identifi-
cation of value tradeoffs was between situations
rather than within one situation. The negotiations
concerned only one issue (situation).
The two values, which the adversaries most fre-
quently expressed – universalism and benevolence –
are part of Schwartz’s value dimension labeled
self-transcendence. In terms of Schwartz’s theory
this should lay the grounds for a not too difficult
trade-off. Yet, most negotiators kept closely to their
initially expressed values and did not move that short
distance to a shared value. Part of the failure was due
to stubbornness to move away from tangible data
points in the case – data points that became the
grounds for initial positions, e.g., three people
would died versus 300 handicapped, non-mobile
people would lose their jobs.
In most of the resolutions egoistic, personal,
hedonistic well-being was not mentioned, and if
mentioned, it was mentioned in initial positions, but
not in the latter stages of the discourse. Religious
values as well did not enter into the discussions. But,
self-respect, classified under the dimension labeled
Self Direction, did appear to be an underlying
influence for many of the negotiators. One reason
may be a fear that abandoning original values would
be seen as a weakness and cause lack of respect from
others, when searching for a new proposal? Ironi-
cally, self-respect could have been turned from an
obstacle to a core shared value if it had been ap-
proached constructively? This would be the case in
face-to-face negotiations as well as in Internet
negotiations.
Analysis of face-to-face negotiations
Only 11 of the 20 face-to-face negotiations were
successfully resolved. The number of statements
constituting the negotiations ranged from 13 to 86
with the median average being 29.
There were many more requests for clarification
in the face-to-face negotiations than in the ones
conducted via the Internet. Most of these requests
came as interruptions to the other party’s presenta-
tion. Interruptions make it difficult to understand
the presenter’s complete argument. The interrupter
tended to focus on a particular point made by the
other party (analogous to focusing on a tree) rather
than on the other party’s total presentation of a
contextual argument (analogous to focusing on a
forest). If the interrupter had waited for the other
590225941.004.png
Face-to-face Versus Internet Negotiations
169
party to finish the presentation, the questioned point
might have been clarified. These interruptions and
the perceived failure on the part of the interrupter to
understand the complete argument of the presenter
may have been the cause of the presenter reiterating
key parts of the presenter’s argument – many more
restatements than found in the Internet negotiations.
Reiteration of statements is most likely the pre-
senter’s attempt to bring about one of Habermas’
four preconditions for successful discourse – that of
mutual comprehension (MC). The presenter’s hope
may be that as soon as the other party comprehends
the logic of the presenter’s argument, the other party
will, at the least, respect the presenter and, at best, be
won over by the argument.
In this particular ethical dilemma two of Haber-
mas’ four preconditions – truth and truthfulness –
should be taken as a given. Both parties were
provided with the same background information
(satisfying the precondition for truth) and, thus,
there were few if any opportunities for deception
(satisfying the precondition for truthfulness.) The
focal points of this negotiation should reduce to the
other two of Habermas’ four preconditions – MC
and Appropriateness (A).
Paraphrasing on the part of the second party
shows that a degree of MC has been attained.
Paraphrasing was more evident in the Internet
negotiations than in the face-to-face negotiations.
That may have occurred because each party had time
to reflect on the other party’s complete argument,
i.e., last statement as well as all preceding statements,
before responding with a paraphrase. This is a ben-
efit of asynchronous Internet negotiations. The
argumentative process in the failed negotiations was
slightly different. In all of the failed Internet nego-
tiations the parties at least attempted to satisfy
Habermas’ precondition of A. They tested alterna-
tive positions. In some of the failed face-to-face
negotiations neither party attempted to explore
other alternatives or A. They just agreed to disagree.
In the face-to-face negotiations there were quite a
few reflective comments at the end, i.e., after a
resolution had been reached or the parties had
agreed to disagree. One could attribute this to the
negotiators’ desire for respect – respect for the logic
behind their decision. The more structured the
negotiation the less emotional it was, and the more
likely that it resulted in a resolution. Structure was
evidenced in the face-to-face negotiations in one of
two ways. First, one of the parties suggested a format
for the subsequent argumentation at the beginning
of the negotiation. Second, one of the parties took
brief notes of what was transpiring during the
negotiation. This helped attain MC and defuse
emotion. But, does the nature of a face to face-to-
face negotiation, as contrasted with an Internet
negotiation, make it more difficult to rationally
deliberate over ethical arguments?
Discussion
Synchronous face-to-face negotiations, while they
allow for time to prepare a strategy before the
negotiation, allow for little time to revise that
strategy during the negotiation. In contrast, asyn-
chronous Internet negotiations allow time to delib-
erate changes of strategy as well as tactics during the
negotiation. The added time can be used not only
for reflection but also for checking one’s own logic
and terminology. The negotiator can then structure
comments so as to preclude anticipated questions
and potential objections.
Additional time for reflection is just one of the
potential benefits of negotiating ethical issues over
the Internet. Other benefits are listed in Table IV.
These benefits might help explain why the first re-
search proposition, ‘‘Negotiations concerning a
business issue with ethical ramifications, conducted
over the Internet, will produce a greater percentage
of successful resolutions than those conducted face-
to-face’’, was supported by this study. But, Internet
negotiation is not without its detriments. Two of the
detriments pointed out by Drake et al. merit serious
attention. One is that most people would rather talk
than pursue the more arduous task of typing com-
ments on a discussion board. The typing task may
motivate negotiators to move too rapidly toward
closure. The second problem is the one most
emphasized by those who favor face-to-face nego-
tiations. It is that non-verbal cues, indicating the
other party’s receptivity to a statement, are stripped
from the message.
It was assumed that the discussion board would
act as an electronic firewall behind which Internet
negotiators would feel comfortable expressing val-
ues. It was also assumed that face-to-face negotiators
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin